Elsevier

Behavioural Brain Research

Volume 181, Issue 2, 6 August 2007, Pages 218-223
Behavioural Brain Research

Research report
Neural substrates underlying evaluation of pain in actions depicted in words

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2007.04.008Get rights and content

Abstract

Previous research has shown that evaluation of pain shown in pictures is mediated by a cortical circuit consisting of the primary and secondary somatosensory cortex (SI and SII), the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and the insula. SI and SII subserve the sensory-discriminative component of pain processing whereas ACC and the insula mediate the affective-motivational aspect of pain processing. The current work investigated the neural correlates of evaluation of pain depicted in words. Subjects were scanned using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) while reading words or phrases depicting painful or neutral actions. Subjects were asked to rate pain intensity of the painful actions depicted in words or counting the number of Chinese characters in the words. Relative to the counting task, rating pain intensity induced activations in SII, the insula, the right middle frontal gyrus, the left superior temporal sulcus and the left middle occipital gyrus. Our results suggest that both the sensory-discriminative and affective-motivational components of the pain matrix are engaged in the processing of pain depicted in words.

Introduction

It is well documented that pain processing is mediated by a neural network consisting of both cortical and subcortical structures. The cortical structures of the pain matrix includes the primary and secondary somatosensory cortex (SI and SII), the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), the insula and prefrontal regions, whereas the subcortical components of the pain matrix includes the thalamus, amygdala and hippocampus, etc. [1], [2]. Nociceptive specific neurons in SI and SII respond only to noxious stimuli [3], [4], [5] and their receptive fields are organized in a somatotopic pattern (particularly in the post-central gurys) [6]. Neuroimaging studies also showed evidence that painful stimulation induced increased hemodynamic responses in SI and SII [7] or resulted in positive covariation between the activity in SI and other brain regions that are known to be involved in pain processing [8]. These findings suggest that SI and SII play an important role in the sensory-discriminative component of pain processing. Recent neuroimaging studies show that while the activity of ACC and the insula can be increased by noxious stimuli [9], [10], illusions evoked by non-noxious stimuli [11] and perception of others in pain [12], [13], [14], [15] also induce activation in ACC and the insula. In addition, the magnitude of ACC activity is correlated with subjective pain-related unpleasantness [9]. These results indicate that ACC and the insula subserve the affective-motivational component of pain processing.

Most of the previous studies of pain processing employed noxious stimuli such as thermal or mechanical stimulation. However, people often encounter ‘painful’ information when noxious stimuli are absent. For example, words or phrases such as “prick” and “hit by a car” are used in novels or newspapers to describe painful situations. To date, we know little about whether the same brain circuits underlying pain processing can also be activated by pain evaluation depicted in words. In particular, it is unclear if both the sensory-discriminative and the affective-motivational components of the pain matrix are involved in evaluation of pain depicted in words. Osaka et al. found that ACC can be activated when subjects were asked to form unpleasant mental images of affective pain corresponding to onomatopoeia words suggestive of subjective pain presented auditorily [16]. The current study extended the previous work by assessing whether evaluation of pain described in words or short phrases presented visually induces activation of the pain matrix. One possibility is that reading words or phrases depicting painful actions is similar to watching painful pictures in generating empathic responses mediated by the pain matrix. Alternatively, words or phrases cannot produce painful situations as vivid as those illustrated in pictures and, thus, cannot activate the neural circuit underlying empathy for pain. We addressed these issues by recording heomodynamic responses from human adults using functional magnetic resonance (fMRI). Brain activity linked to rating pain intensity of actions described in Chinese words or phrases presented visually was contrasted with reading words or phrases describing neutral actions so as to identify whether the somatosensory cortex and other brain areas such as ACC and insula are involved in evaluation of pain depicted in words. We also compared the tasks of counting the number of Chinese characters in painful and neutral words to assess the necessity of the rating task in activation of the pain matrix.

Section snippets

Subjects

Ten healthy subjects (3 males and 7 females) aged between 20 and 27 years (mean ± S.D. = 21.6 ± 2.01) participated in this study as paid volunteers. All subjects had no neurological or psychiatric history. All were right-handed, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and were naïve to the purpose of this study. Informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to scanning. This study was approved by the local ethic committee from the Department of Psychology at Peking University.

Stimuli and procedure

The

Behavioral performance

There was no significant difference between the percentages of painful words rated as mildly painful and extremely painful (46.5 ± 10.42% versus 53.5 ± 10.42%, t(9) = 1.062; p > 0.05) in the pain rating task. Error rates for judging the number of Chinese characters in the words were below 1.0%.

fMRI results

Relative to the task of counting neutral words, rating painful words revealed significant activations in the right anterior insula, the frontal gyrus bilaterally, the left superior temporal sulcus, the left SII,

Dicsussion

This study examined to what extent the pain matrix can be activated by evaluation of pain intensity of actions described in words or short phrases presented visually. In particular, we investigated whether the neural correlates of pain evaluation of painful actions depicted in words are different from those induced by perception of painful pictures [12], [13], [14], [15]. We found, by recording heomodynamic responses using fMRI, that rating pain intensity of actions depicted in words or short

Acknowledgement

This work was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China (project 30630025).

References (36)

  • P.A. Gelnar et al.

    A comparative fMRI study of cortical representations for thermal painful, vibrotactile and motor performance tasks

    Neuroimage

    (1999)
  • R. Peyron et al.

    Role of operuloinsular cortices in human pain processing: converging evidence from PET, fMRI, dipole modeling and intracerebral recordings of evoked potentials

    Neuroimage

    (2002)
  • C.A. Porro et al.

    Functional activity mapping of the mesial hemispheric wall during anticipation of pain

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • P. Petrovic et al.

    Pain-related cerebral activation is altered by a distracting cognitive task

    Pain

    (2000)
  • D.D. Price

    Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain

    Science

    (2000)
  • D.R. Kenshalo et al.

    Responses of primate SI cortical neurons to noxious stimuli

    J Neurophysiol

    (1983)
  • C.J. Robinson et al.

    Somatic submodality distribution within the second somatosensory (SII), 7b, retroinsular, postauditory and granular insular cortical areas of M. fascicularis

    J Comp Neurol

    (1980)
  • Y. Lamour et al.

    Rat somatosensory (SmI) cortex: I. Characteristics of neuronal responses to noxious stimulation and comparison with responses to non-noxious stimulation

    Exp Brain Res

    (1983)
  • Cited by (36)

    • Posterior P1 and early frontal negativity reflect developmental changes in attentional distraction during adolescence

      2014, Brain and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      For the Count (distraction) trials, the participants were instructed to silently count backwards from 100 by three, i.e., 100, 97, 94, etc. The participants always viewed the image on the screen during the distraction task (Gu & Han, 2007; Richter, Eck, Straube, Miltner, & Weiss, 2010; Zack, Sharpley, Dent, & Stacy, 2009). For all of the trials, the participants were instructed to keep their eyes on the screen and to avoid diverting their gaze away from the emotionally salient aspects of these images.

    • Distraction reduces theta synchronization in emotion regulation during adolescence

      2013, Neuroscience Letters
      Citation Excerpt :

      The present study examined if theta ERS or ERD reflects the developmental changes in attentional distraction during adolescence. EEG data were collected from 48 adolescents and young adults as they performed a distraction task [16,27] while viewing affective pictures from the Chinese affective picture system (CAPS) [7]. Afterwards, the arousal of each picture was rated.

    • Distinct roles of the 'Shared Pain' and 'Theory of Mind' networks in processing others' emotional suffering

      2012, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      The correspondence between these regions, and the previously identified Shared Pain regions, was corroborated by functional region of interest analyses. For the most part, our results show striking convergence with the only previous study to compare neural responses to verbal descriptions of physically painful and non-painful events (Gu & Han, 2007b). In that study, participants read one- or two-character long descriptions of events (in Chinese), and evaluated how painful the event would be.

    • Subliminal presentation of other faces (but not own face) primes behavioral and evoked cortical processing of empathy for pain

      2011, Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      The contextual dependence effects of N1 and P3 elicited in this study are consistent with the contextual malleability of empathy process. Neuroimaging studies have shown that the perception of pain in others is modulated by attentional demands (Fan and Han, 2008; Gun and Han, 2007), social relationship (Cheng et al., 2010; Singer et al., 2006), cognitive appraisal (Lamm et al., 2007), and implicit attitudes towards others (Decety et al., 2010a). In the same line, ERP studies of empathy for pain have shown early modulation by contextual reality of stimuli, and late modulation based on cognitive regulatory and task demands (Fan and Han, 2008; Han et al., 2008; Decety et al., 2010c; Li and Han, 2010).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text